Page 1 of 9
Agricultural and Forest Entomology (2012), 14, 286–294 DOI: 10.1111/j.1461-9563.2012.00568.x
The role of vision in the host orientation behaviour of Hylobius
warreni
Laura A. Machial, B. Staffan Lindgren and Brian H. Aukema∗
Natural Resources and Environmental Studies Institute, University of Northern British Columbia, 3333 University Way, Prince George, British
Columbia V2N 4Z9, Canada, and ∗Department of Entomology, University of Minnesota, 217 Hodson Hall, 1980 Folwell Avenue, St Paul, MN
55118, U.S.A.
Abstract 1 Visual stimuli, often in combination with olfactory stimuli, are frequently important
components of host selection by forest-dwelling phytophagous insects.
2 Warren root collar weevil Hylobius warreni Wood (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) is a
native insect in western Canada, where larvae feed primarily on lodgepole pine Pinus
contorta and can girdle and kill young trees. This weevil is an emerging problem in
areas heavily impacted by mountain pine beetle Dendroctonus ponderosae Hopkins.
3 No olfactory attractants have been identified for this insect, making monitoring and
management difficult. Thus, we investigated the role of vision in the host-finding
behaviour of Warren root collar weevil in the absence of known olfactory cues.
4 We conducted three experiments in field enclosure plots aiming to characterize
aspects of host-finding behaviour by adult Warren root collar weevil.
5 We found that both male and female weevils were readily attracted to vertical plastic
silhouettes in the shape of a trunk, crown or tree at distances of less than 4 m. This
pattern of attraction persisted over 2 years in two slightly different study designs.
Blinding the insects removed their ability to orient to these silhouettes, indicating
that host-finding behaviour has a strong visual component. The use of different
colour trunks and crowns (black, white and green) did not change the patterns of
attraction of the insects to the silhouettes.
6 Exploiting visual attraction in this walking insect may present a new management
tool in forest protection strategies.
Keywords Host selection, host-finding behaviour, silhouettes, vision, Warren root
collar weevil.
Introduction
Host selection by phytophagous insects is characterized by
two activities: host location and host assessment (Dethier,
1983). Locating hosts requires finding appropriate habitat and
then identifying a host plant, potentially among other nonhost
vegetation (Dethier, 1983; Huber et al., 2000; Raffa, 2001;
Bernays, 2003). Insects utilize various strategies to maximize
their chances of encountering an appropriate plant, such as
increasing activity, moving randomly, turning frequently and/or
responding to various host stimuli with an orderly sequence of
behaviour patterns (Dethier, 1983). Olfactory and visual stimuli
are both perceived as attractive cues by many insects when
Correspondence: Laura Machial. Tel.: (651) 408-4239;
fax: (612) 625-5299; e-mail: lauramachial@gmail.com
initially locating plants on which to feed and oviposit (Bernays
& Chapman, 1994; Bernays, 2003).
Although both types of cues have been shown to be
integrated by phytophagous insects, including many types of
forest-dwelling arthropods (Borden et al., 1985; Strom et al.,
1999, 2001; Bjorklund ̈ et al., 2005; Campbell & Borden, 2006a,
b), olfactory cues are frequently viewed as the more important.
Not unexpectedly, the role of olfaction has received the most
attention in studies of how insects select their hosts. The use of
visual stimuli in detecting plants has been reported for several
species (Prokopy & Owens, 1983; Reeves, 2011), although
few insects were found to use visual cues in the absence of
olfactory cues (Stenberg & Ericson, 2007; Reeves et al., 2009;
Reeves, 2011).
Warren root collar weevil Hylobius warreni Wood
(Coleoptera: Curculionidae) is a phytophagous insect that is
© 2012 The Authors
Agricultural and Forest Entomology © 2012 The Royal Entomological Society
Page 2 of 9
Host-finding behaviour of H. warreni 287
native throughout Canada’s boreal forests (Cerezke, 1994).
Adult weevils can reach 5 years of age. The weevils cannot fly,
and thus they encounter hosts as they walk along the forest floor
(Grant, 1966; Cerezke, 1994). In British Columbia, Canada, the
weevil’s primary host is lodgepole pine Pinus contorta var. lat- ifolia (Warren, 1956; Wood, 1957; Grant, 1966). Adults ascend
conifer trees at dusk to feed on the branches, bark and needles.
Feeding by adult Warren root collar weevil typically does not
cause tree mortality (Warren, 1956; Warner, 1966). Larval feed- ing, however, may cut into the cambial and xylem tissues of the
host’s large lateral roots or root collar (Warren, 1956; Cerezke,
1994). In young trees, complete girdling can be accomplished
by as few as one to three larvae. In trees with larger diameters,
many more weevils are required to complete girdling, although
insects rarely occur at such high densities. Warren root collar
weevils can cause mortality in stands as old as 30 years of age,
although peak mortality occurs in stands aged 5–10 years of
age (Cerezke, 1994).
The current epidemic of mountain pine beetle Dendroctonus
ponderosae Hopkins has killed over 630 million m3 of mature
lodgepole pine in British Columbia and Alberta since 1998 and
is predicted to kill approximately 67% of mature pine in British
Columbia by 2016 (Aukema et al., 2006; Walton, 2010). As a
result, salvage logging and reforestation has begun to shift the
age structure and species composition of large areas of forest.
Recently, studies have reported increased mortality to young
trees caused by Warren root collar weevil (Robert & Lindgren,
2006), especially in areas where harvested and reforested lands
are located adjacent to stands of mature lodgepole pine heavily
affected by mountain pine beetle where the timber has not
been removed. Such patterns are consistent with the hypothesis
that weevils are migrating from forests with depleted host
pools to replanted areas in search of new hosts (Klingenberg
et al., 2010b).
Despite the increased importance of the insect, little is
known about the host selection mechanisms of Warren root
collar weevil. In closely-related species of Hylobius, volatiles
of host terpenes and ethanol play prominent roles in host
attraction (Tilles et al., 1986a, b; Raffa & Hunt, 1988).
Attraction to such compounds has been exploited to increase
trapping efficiency and has greatly aided the development
of management plans for pine weevils (Nordlander, 1987;
Raffa & Hunt, 1988; Hunt & Raffa, 1989; Rieske & Raffa,
1993). Studies aiming to determine whether Warren root
collar weevils are similarly attracted to host volatiles have
yielded inconclusive results (K. Sambaraju and B. S. Lindgren,
personal communication; Duke & Lindgren, 2006). In these
field and laboratory olfactometer studies, weevils failed to
show responses to chemical stimuli such as monoterpene
components that characterize lodgepole pine. Moreover, no
clear links have been found between tree chemistry and
susceptibility to attack by Warren root collar weevil (Duke &
Lindgren, 2006).
It is possible that vision plays a prominent role in host
orientation for Warren root collar weevil. In a review on the
ecology, behaviour and management of the weevil, Cerezke
(1994) commented that host selection was probably visual
in nature and thus influenced by silhouettes. This hypothesis
was proposed after noticing that the capture frequency of
adult weevils was positively correlated with an increasing tree
diameter (Cerezke, 1994). Moreover, a number of studies have
observed that weevils orient and move toward two-dimensional
silhouettes in laboratory settings (Hoover, 2000; Horning &
Lindgren, 2002). These findings are consistent with those found
for other conifer-feeding insects, including other pine weevil
species that integrate visual cues in the presence of olfactory
cues. For example, the pales weevil H. pales Herbst is more
attracted to traps baited with ethanol and turpentine that have
white silhouettes than to traps with black or green silhouettes
(Hunt & Raffa, 1991).
The present study aimed to provide additional information
concerning the role of vision in the host-finding behavior of
Warren root collar weevil. We investigated the hypothesis that
vision plays a role in initial steps of the weevils’ host finding
by exploring three questions. First, are weevils attracted to
visual cues? If so, does blinding the insects remove their host- finding ability? Finally, does colour affect locomotor response
to vertical silhouettes?
Materials and methods
Site set-up
During the summers of 2009 and 2010, we conducted
three experiments aiming to determine the role of vision in
host selection by Warren root collar weevil. In 2009, host- seeking experiments were conducted in 16 square, outdoor
bioassay plots measuring 1.5 × 1.5 m in an approximately
20 × 20 m area with bentgrass, Agrostis sp., vegetation in
Prince George, British Columbia, Canada (53◦
51
43.2N,
122◦
45
39.6W). Each plot was surrounded by a 1-m tall
polypropylene mesh wrapped around wooden corner stakes.
The bottom 15 cm of the mesh was lined using duct tape
with a slippery fluorocarbon polymer (2009: AD1070, AGC
Chemicals Americas, Inc., Bayonne, New Jersey; 2010: Teflon
PTFE, DuPont, Wilmington, Delaware) to prevent the weevils
from climbing the mesh and escaping from the plot (Bjorklund, ̈
2009). In addition, the bottom of the mesh was pinned to the
ground with nails (length 5 cm) as a further measure to prevent
weevil escape. The mesh and stakes were painted white to
maximize the contrast of the silhouette treatment, inside the
plot, against the background. Plots were located a minimum of
2 m apart.
Within each plot, two plastic flower pots (diameter 25 cm,
depth 10 cm) were installed as pitfall traps. The traps were
located 15 cm from the north and south sides of each plot such
that the distance between the centres of the traps was 95 cm
(Fig. 1A). Pitfall traps were placed so that their tops were flush
with the ground. The top 4 cm inside the pitfall traps was coated
with polymer to prevent captured weevils from climbing the
sides and escaping.
In 2010, the study site was located at the Prince George
Tree Improvement Station (53◦
46
18N, 122◦
43
4W). This
permitted enlargement of the plots to 2 × 4 m, spacing centres
of the two pitfall traps in each arena 1 m from the fencing
at both sides of the plot, and increasing the distance between
centres of pitfall traps within a plot to 2 m (Fig. 1B). We judged
this distance of 2 m to be more representative of the mean
© 2012 The Authors
Agricultural and Forest Entomology © 2012 The Royal Entomological Society, Agricultural and Forest Entomology, 14, 286–294
Page 3 of 9
288 L. A. Machial et al.
(A) (B)
Figure 1 Schematic diagram of the design of outdoor bioassay plots
used to investigate the role of vision in initial steps of adult Warren root
collar weevils’ host finding. Each plot had a treatment trap and an empty
trap, except the control treatment, which had two empty traps. (A) Layout
of 16 bioassay plots used in 2009 in Prince George, British Columbia,
Canada (53◦
51
43.2N, 122◦
45
39.6W). (B) Layout of 20 bioassay
plots used in 2010 at the Prince George Tree Improvement Station,
Prince George, British Columbia, Canada (53◦
46
18N, 122◦
43
4W).
Five weevils were released per plot.
distance that Warren root collar weevils can move in a night
(Cerezke, 1994). We constructed 20 plots in 2010.
In both years, we tested weevil responses by erecting a
dummy host-tree, which provided a visual stimulus in the form
of a vertical silhouette in one of the two pitfall traps, chosen
randomly by a coin toss, within each arena. Different types of
silhouettes were used to emulate different potential hosts, and
thus defined the experimental treatment. The specific treatments
are described further below. The silhouette was erected in
the middle of the pitfall trap such that it did not contact the
ground outside of the trap, preventing the insects from climbing
it without first entering the pitfall trap. The treatments were
randomly assigned to the plots. Control treatments did not
receive a silhouette (i.e. both pitfall traps were empty in those
arenas).
Study organisms
Bjorklund funnel traps were made using tar paper (GAP ̈
Roofing Black #30 Roofing Felt, GAP Roofing, Inc., Pryor,
Oklahoma) and installed on 167 lodgepole pine trees in a
10–20-year-old lodgepole pine stand in 2009, and on 602 trees
in similar stands (7–25 years) in 2010 near Prince George,
British Columbia, Canada (53◦
55
N, 122◦
49
W) in accordance
with the protocol previously described by Bjorklund (2009). ̈
In 2009, there were two sampling periods, whereas, in 2010,
there was only a single, longer, sampling period. The traps
were checked each morning, and yielded 324 adult weevils on
11 May to 17 June and 13–28 July 2009, and 519 adult weevils
on 26 April to 18 August, 2010.
Captured weevils were retained in groups of eight in square
plastic containers (15 × 15 × 5 cm) with a piece of moist
paper towel and a few small lodgepole pine branches for food
(Toivonen & Viiri, 2006; Hopkins et al., 2009). Food was
changed twice per week, as required. The weevils were stored
in an environmental growth chamber under a LD 12 : 12 h
photocycle at 8 ◦
C and 75% relative humidity. The temperature
was set at 8 ◦
C to slow weevil metabolism (Toivonen &
Viiri, 2006).
The sex of the weevils was determined using two non- invasive techniques described previously by Ohrn ̈ et al. (2008).
Internal markings on the eighth sternite were also examined to
increase confidence in the accuracy of the identification (G. R.
Hopkins, M. D. Klingenberg and B. H. Aukema, unpublished
data). Male and female weevils were then divided into separate
plastic containers, again in groups of eight.
Experimental trials
In both years, all weevils utilized for an experiment had been
captured that season. We marked each insect because the
recapture rate of weevils released was not 100% and plots were
reused for numerous trials. Marking was conducted by etching
the elytra with a rotary drill as described by Klingenberg et al.
(2010a), comprising a technique that has been successfully
used on other ground-dwelling Coleoptera (Winder, 2004).
Etchings were then filled in with nontoxic latex-based paint
(Citadel Colour, U.K.) to accentuate markings (Klingenberg
et al., 2010a).
Before each experimental trial, weevils were placed in
clean plastic containers without food for a period of 24 h.
Containers were kept at ambient temperatures and exposed to
the natural photoperiod in the range 12.8–14.2 h/day in 2009
to 14.3–17.0 h/day in 2010 depending on the season (National
Research Council of Canada, 2011). For each experiment, five
weevils were released along the centre line; once per plot per
trial. This density reflects the highest weevil density commonly
seen on single hosts in 20–25-year stands (Cerezke, 1994).
Each trial ran 60 h. Pitfall traps were checked every 12 h for
the duration of the trial. When a weevil was found in a pitfall
trap, the replicate and treatment were recorded. The weevil
was subsequently removed from the experiment. Insects were
not reused.
Experiment 1: are weevils attracted to visual cues?
This experiment investigated the response of Warren root
collar weevils to four silhouette treatments: crown, trunk, tree
(crown + trunk) and control. The crown treatment consisted
of a plastic Christmas tree (height 138 cm, trunk diameter
3 cm). The trunk treatment consisted of a piece of acrylonitrile
butadiene styrene (ABS) pipe (height 90 cm, diameter 10 cm).
The tree treatment consisted of a piece of ABS pipe (height
90 cm, diameter 10 cm), with a plastic Christmas tree (height
138 cm) inserted to give the appearance of the pipe being
the trunk of the tree. The combined height of the ABS pipe
© 2012 The Authors
Agricultural and Forest Entomology © 2012 The Royal Entomological Society, Agricultural and Forest Entomology, 14, 286–294
Page 4 of 9
Host-finding behaviour of H. warreni 289
and Christmas tree was approximately 188 cm as a result
of part of the Christmas tree being inserted into the ABS
pipe. The control treatment had no silhouettes in either pitfall
trap. In 2009, artificial Christmas trees were procured from
garage sales in the Prince George metropolitan area. In 2010,
the artificial trees were identical ‘Canadian pine’ specimens
(original height 200 cm, crown height 104.1 cm), which were
purchased from Wal-Mart (Canada). Despite the name, the trees
had a visual appearance of a unique hybrid of pine and spruce;
hosts acceptable to H. warreni. The trees had seven rows of six
to eight branches. The branches decreased in length from the
bottom of the tree to the top; the largest branches were 53 cm
and the shortest were 27 cm. The branches had 10–16 ‘shoots’,
and the shoots were covered in 3-cm long plastic ‘needles’. All
plastic trees were unboxed and exposed to the air for over 48 h
before use in experiments, although no associated odours were
detectable to the human nose during the experiment set-up.
Replicated trials were conducted from 25 to 28 August and
29 to 31 August 2009 and from 17 to 19 June and 23 to 25
June, 2010.
In 2009, a logistic mixed-effects analysis of variance was
used to examine the effect of silhouette treatments on the
number of weevils captured in control versus pitfall trap
treatments (a binary response). Plot was the unit of replication.
Fixed effects included terms for crown and trunk, as well
as their interaction, whereas a term for plot was modelled
as a random effect. Where significant differences between
treatments existed (using α = 0.05), means comparisons were
performed using protected t-tests (Carmer & Swanson, 1973).
In addition, a chi-square contingency analysis was conducted
to determine whether there was a difference in the number of
weevils caught in pitfall traps baited with the different treatment
types. In 2010, with expanded plot size, no weevils were caught
in empty traps within the treatment plots (see Results). A
lack of weevils in these traps created an analytical challenge
in estimating the proportion of weevils attracted to control
versus treatments and the standard errors of those estimates
in a binomial model framework because only treatments with a
silhouette elicited positive responses. Hence, in 2010, we used
chi-square contingency analysis to determine whether there
was a difference in the number of weevils caught in pitfall
traps with the different treatment types. All data analyses were
performed in r, version 2.12.2 (R Development Core Team,
2011).
Experiment 2: does blinding remove host-finding ability?
This experiment investigated whether vision was a mechanism
used by Warren root collar weevil in host orientation. Each
plot was set up with two pitfall traps as described for the
2010 experiments above. One pitfall trap contained an artificial
Christmas tree crown inserted into an ABS pipe to serve as
a tree silhouette (tree treatment); the other was left empty
(control). Five weevils were released in each of the 20 bioassay
plots as described previously. In half the plots, blinded weevils
were released; in the other half, nonblinded weevils were
released.
Weevils were blinded using nontoxic Elmer’s All Ceramic
and Glass Cement mixed with lamp black nontoxic acrylic paint
(Americana, Elmery’s, Columbus, Ohio). Blinding insects by
applying paint to their eyes has confirmed use of vision in
silhouette location by common field grasshoppers Chorthippus
brunneus Thunberg (Kral, 2008), as well as prey-finding
behaviour by a number of predaceous insects (Rossel, 1986;
Awan et al., 1989; Claver & Ambrose, 2001). The glue–paint
mixture was applied to the weevils’ eyes using a synthetic 000
S/H round paintbrush (Winsor & Newton University Series,
U.K). Trials were run from 12 to 14 September, 2009, 11 to
13 June, 2010 and 6 to 8 July, 2010.
In 2009, a likelihood ratio test (G-test) was used to examine
the effect of blinding on the number of weevils found in empty
pitfall traps versus pitfall traps underneath tree silhouettes. The
test statistic of the G-test is distributed according to a chi-square
distribution, and can be used when expected values are less than
five in the contingency table (Gotelli & Ellison, 2004).
As in Experment 1, the larger plots in 2010 resulted in very
few insects being captured in the empty controls (see Results).
Hence, we focused only on insects captured in pitfall traps with
a tree silhouette. When inspecting weevils post-capture under
a dissecting microscope, we found that some of the weevils’
eyes were only partially covered with paint, either as a result of
burrowing activity or scratching with tarsi. Because we could
not conclusively confirm that all weevils were 100% blinded,
we analyzed the likelihood of a weevil being captured in a
trap with a tree silhouette as a function of vision impaired by
paint versus nonblinded. In this logistic regression analysis, the
statistical test associated with estimate of the intercept divided
by its standard error yields a Z-statistic reflecting a test of
whether the probability of recovering a nonblinded weevil in a
silhouette baited trap was significantly different from 50% (i.e.
reflecting random movement) [the test of whether the intercept
was significantly different from zero, back-transformed by the
logit link function, exp0/(1 + exp0), reflects Ho: P(recovered
specimen has paint on its eyes) = 0.50]. Again, a term for
plot was included as a random effect. Data analysis was
performed in r, version 2.12.2 (R Development Core Team,
2011).
Experiment 3: are weevils attracted to colour?
Experiments looking at the effects of white/black trunks and
white/green crowns on Warren root collar weevil host selection
were conducted during the summer of 2010. Four treatments
were investigated. The treatments were ‘trees’ constructed with
a white trunk and a green crown, a white trunk and a white
crown, a black trunk and a white crown, and a black trunk and
a green crown. The white trunks were composed of polyvinyl
chloride (PVC) pipe (height 90 cm, diameter 10 cm), and black
trunks were composed of similar ABS pipe. The white crowns
were painted twice with white acrylic, latex, exterior, flat
paint (BEHR Premium Plus Ultra Pure White Co., Santa Ana,
California). The crowns of the green artificial Christmas trees
were left unpainted. Again, each treatment was characterized by
a silhouette placed into a pitfall trap opposing an empty pitfall
trap in the other side of the enclosure plot. Two replicates of
each of the four treatments were conducted at a time. Four trials
were conducted: 3–5 August, 9–11 August, 12–14 August and
21–23 August, 2010.
© 2012 The Authors
Agricultural and Forest Entomology © 2012 The Royal Entomological Society, Agricultural and Forest Entomology, 14, 286–294
Page 5 of 9
290 L. A. Machial et al.
Before carrying out the present study, we conducted a
preliminary study investigating the effects of paint on weevil
behaviour. In that experiment, weevils were given a choice
between two similar tree silhouettes: one crown was painted
with a matching green colour of the original plastic branches,
whereas the other was left unpainted (= green). A logistic
regression model was used to determine whether paint affected
the number of weevils caught in the pitfall traps surrounding
both trees) (Z = −1.24, P = 0.215. Thus, we concluded that
paint, at least in the tree crowns, did not affect weevil behaviour
(L. Machial, unpublished data).
As a result of the tendency of weevils to avoid empty pitfall
traps in the larger plots in 2010, we again focused solely on
the number of weevils caught in pitfall traps surrounding the
four, coloured silhouettes. A chi-square contingency analysis
was conducted to determine whether there was a difference
among treatments. Data analysis was performed in r, version
2.12.2 (R Development Core Team, 2011).
Results
Experiment 1: are weevils attracted to visual cues?
Weevils demonstrated clear attraction to vertical silhouettes
in both 2009 and 2010, although the type of silhouette
eliciting attraction was not necessarily consistent between years
(Fig. 2). For example, in 2009, the proportion of weevils
falling into the two pitfall traps in the control plots was not
significantly different from 50% (i.e. a random pattern of
dispersion in the absence of vertical silhouettes). The crown
and trunk treatments did not differ from controls, although the
tree treatment that combined the trunk and crown silhouette
captured significantly more weevils than the associated empty
trap (F1,30 = 5.48; P = 0.0259; Fig. 2A). In addition, when
the proportion of weevils found in pitfall traps baited with
silhouettes was directly compared, more weevils were found
in pitfall traps baited with tree silhouettes than in crown,
trunk or the empty control pitfall trap (χ2 = 10, d.f. = 3,
P = 0.0186).
In 2010, proportionately more weevils were found in the
pitfall traps surrounding crown, trunk and tree silhouettes
than in the empty control pitfall traps (χ2 = 13.7, d.f. = 3,
P = 0.004; Fig. 2B). Indeed, not a single weevil was captured
in the empty trap in any of the plots containing a silhouette.
There was no preference for tree silhouettes over crown or
trunk silhouettes (χ2 = 0.316, d.f. = 2, P = 0.85). Sex did
not affect host-orientation behaviour because there was no
difference in the response of male and female weevils to various
silhouettes for either year (2009: F1,29 = 0.22; P = 0.64; 2010:
χ2 = 4.83, d.f. = 3, P = 0.69).
Experiment 2: does blinding remove host-finding ability
of weevils?
Application of paint to eyes of the insects altered their attraction
to vertical silhouettes. In a preliminary experiment conducted
in 2009, 17 of the 40 weevils originally released into the plots
were recaptured in the pitfall traps. The proportions found in the
(A)
(B)
Figure 2 Total number of weevils of Warren root collar weevils captured
in pitfall traps in each of four silhouette treatments; tree, crown,
trunk and control. Each plot had a treatment trap and an empty
trap, except the control treatment, which had two empty traps.
(A) Experiments were conducted from 25 to 31 August 2009 (n = 40
weevils per treatment) (five insects × four replicates × two trials). An
asterisk indicates a statistically significant difference in proportions
(F1,30 = 5.48; P = 0.0259). (B) Experiments were conducted from 17
to 25 June 2010 (n = 50 weevils per treatment) (five insects × five
replicates × two trials). An asterisk indicates a statistically significant
number of insects trapped in silhouette treatments versus control
(χ2 = 13.7, d.f. = 3, P = 0.004).
treatment versus empty controls differed significantly among
blinded and nonblinded insects (Gadjusted = 5.35, d.f. = 1, P =
0.0207; Fig. 3). Of the 17 weevils recaptured, seven were
blinded and 10 were not. The seven blinded weevils were found
in both empty pitfall traps (n = 3) and those baited with a tree
silhouette (n = 4). By contrast, all 10 of the nonblinded weevils
were found in pitfall traps with silhouettes. This pattern of host- location persisted in the more robust experiment conducted in
2010.
In 2010, only three weevils of the 100 released over the
course of the assays were found in the empty control. Fifty- three of them were found in the traps beneath tree silhouettes.
Insects found in these traps were more frequently nonblinded
(84.9%) versus blinded (15.1%) (Z = −4.52, P < 0.0001;
Fig. 4).
© 2012 The Authors
Agricultural and Forest Entomology © 2012 The Royal Entomological Society, Agricultural and Forest Entomology, 14, 286–294
Page 6 of 9
Host-finding behaviour of H. warreni 291
Figure 3 Total number of blinded and nonblinded Warren root collar
weevils captured in empty pitfall traps and pitfall traps with tree
silhouettes. Each plot had a treatment trap and an empty trap, except
the control treatment, which had two empty traps. Experiments were
conducted from 12 to 14 September 2009 (n = 20 weevils per treatment)
(five insects × four replicates × two trials). An asterisk indicates a
statistically significant difference in proportions (Gadjusted = 5.35, d.f. = 1,
P = 0.0207).
*
50
40
30
Total Number of Weevils Captured
20
10
Vision Impaired by Paint Non-Blinded
0
Figure 4 Total number of Warren root collar weevils with vision impaired
by paint versus nonblinded controls captured in pitfall traps with tree
silhouettes. Experiments were conducted from 11 to 13 June and
6 to 8 July 2010 (n = 50 weevils per treatment) (five insects × five
replicates × two trials). An asterisk indicates a statistical significance
difference from the other treatment (Z = −4.52, P < 0.0001).
Experiment 3: are weevils attracted to colour?
The attraction of weevils to silhouettes did not change when
colours of the trunk and crown portions were manipulated.
Only two out of 54 captured weevils were found in the empty
control (Fig. 5). The insects demonstrated equal attraction to
silhouettes composed of combinations of white/green crowns
and white/black trunks (χ2 = 0.4615, d.f. = 3, P = 0.93).
Discussion
The finding that vision plays an important role in host finding
behaviour by Warren root collar weevil is consistent with
Figure 5 Total number of Warren root collar weevils captured in arena
choice bioassays with a control pitfall trap and a pitfall trap baited
with one of four silhouette treatments: green crown/black trunk, green
crown/white trunk, white crown/black trunk and white crown/white trunk
no significant difference among silhouette treatments; (χ2 = 0.4615,
d.f. = 3, P = 0.93). Experiments were conducted from 3 to 23 August
2010 (n = 40 weevils per treatment) (five insects × two replicates × four
trials).
behaviour observed in several other wood-boring insects that
integrate visual cues of vertical silhouettes with other cues
(Hunt & Raffa, 1991; Strom et al., 1999; De Groot & Nott,
2001; Strom et al., 2001; Goyer et al., 2004; Campbell &
Borden, 2006a, b; Campbell & Borden, 2009; Reeves, 2011)
The strong role of vision in host orientation by Warren root
collar weevil in the absence of any discernible chemical stimuli
to date, however, is rare among root-boring insects. Among
species of Hylobius, attraction to odourless visual stimuli has
only been previously noted in large pine weevil Hylobius
abietis L. (Bjorklund ̈ et al., 2005). In most insects, responses
to olfactory cues play a dominant role in host selection, and
responses to visual cues only occur when appropriate chemical
cues are present (Bernays & Chapman, 1994; Bernays, 2003).
When searching for nondamaged conifer hosts, H. abietis is
equally attracted to traps baited solely with chemicals or solely
with visual stimuli; however, the strongest attraction is to traps
baited with both chemical and visual stimuli (Bjorklund ̈ et al.,
2005). The distinctive response of Warren root collar weevil
adults to visual stimuli in the absence of chemical stimuli
may be explained, in part, by this insect’s range of host plants
and limited dispersal capabilities associated with its life-history
strategy and mode of movement.
Warren root collar weevils are oligophagous, feeding on a
variety of hosts in the Pinaceae family, including species of
Pinus, Picea, Abies, Larix and Tsuga (Warren, 1956; Wood,
1957; Whitney, 1961; Wood & Van Sickle, 1989; Cerezke,
1994; Hopkins et al., 2009). Oligophagous and monophagous
insects tend to be visual specialists compared with polyphagous
insects because plants within the same family are more likely
to have similar morphologies than plants found in different
© 2012 The Authors
Agricultural and Forest Entomology © 2012 The Royal Entomological Society, Agricultural and Forest Entomology, 14, 286–294
Page 7 of 9
292 L. A. Machial et al.
families (Prokopy & Owens, 1978). Plants showing similar
morphologies allow the insects that feed on them to develop
specific search images that aid in host location (Prokopy &
Owens, 1978, 1983; Aluja & Prokopy, 1993; Stenberg &
Ericson, 2007).
Visual cues are often important in short range host selection,
from distances of a few centimetres up to 10 m (VanderSar
& Borden, 1977; Bernays, 2003). Movement by adult Warren
root collar weevils falls within this range because the insects
traverse a mean distance of up to 2 m per night (Cerezke, 1994;
Klingenberg et al., 2010a; Machial et al., 2012). Flightless
insects may not be as dependent upon long-distance (i.e.
chemical) cues if the insects typically exist within host pools not
ephemeral in space and time. For example, bark beetles require
olfactory capabilities to process a cacophony of competing host- and nonhost volatiles in flight as they seek either new live trees
(in ‘aggressive’ tree-killing species) or stressed and weakened
hosts (in ‘secondary’ species) (Huber et al., 2000; Raffa, 2001).
By contrast, Warren root collar weevil can persist for more
than one generation on a mature coniferous tree without killing
the host (Cerezke, 1994), and many suitable hosts of various
species are often found together in a coniferous forest. After
using visual stimuli to locate and arrive at a potential host, other
sensory cues, such as tactile and gustatory stimuli, are likely
to be incorporated into the host selection process (Bernays
& Chapman, 1994). Exploitation of chemical cues for host
location is likely critical for larval life stages, similar to
H. abietis (Nordenhem & Nordlander, 1994; Nordlander et al.,
1997) because female Warren root collar weevil tend to be egg
scatterers that deposit their eggs near potential hosts.
Our experiments were not designed to characterize seasonal
variations in responses of adult Warren root collar weevils
to host cues. Seasonal variations can result from changes
in weevil age and reproductive status, causing shifts in
the insects’ biological requirements (Nordenhem & Eidmann,
1991; Hoffman et al., 1997). For example, early in the season,
the insects may be searching for hosts on which to feed before
mating, whereas, late in the season, females may be looking for
hosts near which to oviposit. Declining discrimination as female
behaviour shifts from feeding to egg scattering (Minkenberg
et al., 1992; Cerezke, 1994; Hopkins et al., 2009) is consistent
with our results from 2009 to 2010 (Fig. 2), in which a higher
level of discrimination among silhouettes in 2010 coincided
with an earlier season assay (June 2010 versus August 2009).
We were cautious when making comparisons between years,
however, because the plots were enlarged in 2010. As such,
the pattern in 2010 could simply have reflected shelter-seeking
rather than a feeding or ovipositional strategy. In preliminary
assays, however, we found that the insects would readily
climb both PVC pipes and artificial Christmas trees, comprising
behaviour more indicative of searching for food than avoiding
predators or inclement weather.
Many insects that feed on conifer trees are more attracted
to black silhouettes than to white ones (Dubbel et al., 1985;
Strom et al., 1999; Campbell & Borden, 2006a, b; Campbell
& Borden, 2009), putatively because black silhouettes more
closely resemble the trunks of host trees, whereas white
silhouettes more closely resemble the trunks of nonhost
angiosperms. Throughout all experiments conducted in the
present study, weevils demonstrated a preference for full
tree silhouettes, regardless of black, white or green colour
combinations. The attraction to silhouettes may have masked
weaker responses to colour because we did not directly
compare colours in a choice assay. Alternatively, we may have
failed to test the most optimal colour to which Warren root
collar weevils are attracted. Because the insects are putatively
nocturnal, future research should focus on colour contrast,
spectral sensitivity and silhouette angle.
Currently, there is no easy and accurate method for esti- mating weevil population sizes in forests or plantations. Tree
mortality may lag the appearance of high numbers of adult
Warren root collar weevils by 2 or 3 years because larvae, the
most damaging life stage to young trees, typically take 2 years
to develop to adults (Cerezke, 1994). Furthermore, infestation
levels are typically higher than what is apparent by mortality
rates (Schroff et al., 2006). As a result, high levels of dam- age may occur before it becomes apparent that populations
of Warren root collar weevil are at critical levels. Our results
suggest that a trap could potentially be developed by placing
nondestructive tree-trunk funnel traps that capture live Warren
root collar weevils known as Bjorklund traps around the base ̈
of plastic vertical silhouettes (Bjorklund, 2009). Using such ̈
apparatuses versus live trees could allow easy placement and
transport within forested areas where they would be most effec- tive, such as within harvested sites to reduce weevil numbers
before replanting, or at the margins of young plantations to
reduce weevil ingress (Klingenberg et al., 2010b).
Acknowledgements
We thank the Prince George Tree Improvement Station for
providing research space. We also extend our gratitude to
Kathryn Berry, Gareth Hopkins, Honey-Marie de la Giroday,
Genny Michiel, Rurik Muenter, Dr Kishan Sambaraju and
Robin Steenweg (University of Northern British Columbia)
for their excellent field and laboratory assistance. Funding for
this project was provided by the Canadian Forest Service, the
University of Minnesota College of Agriculture, Food, and
Natural Resources, an NSERC Canada Graduate Scholarship
and a UNBC Research Seed Grant to L.A.M., and an NSERC
Discovery Grant to B.H.A. E. B. Radcliffe (University of
Minnesota), Lynne Rieske-Kinney (University of Kentucky)
and five anonymous reviewers provided valuable comments on
earlier drafts of this manuscript.
References
Aluja, M. & Prokopy, R.J. (1993) Host odor and visual stimulus
interaction during intratree host finding behaviour of Rhagoletis
pomonella flies. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 19, 2671–2696.
Aukema, B.H., Carroll, A.L., Zhu, J., Raffa, K.F., Sickley, T.A. &
Taylor, S.W. (2006) Landscape level analysis of mountain pine
beetle in British Columbia, Canada: spatiotemporal development
and spatial synchrony within the present outbreak. Ecography, 29,
427–441.
Awan, M.S., Wilson, L.T. & Hoffmann, M.P. (1989) Prey location by
Oechalia schellembergii. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata,
51, 225–231.
© 2012 The Authors
Agricultural and Forest Entomology © 2012 The Royal Entomological Society, Agricultural and Forest Entomology, 14, 286–294
Page 8 of 9
Host-finding behaviour of H. warreni 293
Bernays, E. (2003) Phytophagous insects. Encyclopedia of Insects (ed.
by V. H. Resh and R. T. Carde), pp. 902–905. Academic Press Inc, ́
San Diego, California.
Bernays, E. & Chapman, R.F. (1994) Host-Plant Selection by Phy- tophagous Insects. Chapman & Hall, Inc., New York, New York.
Bjorklund, N. (2009) Non-destructive tree-trunk funnel trap for ̈
capturing Hylobius warreni (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) ascending
stems of trees. Canadian Entomologist, 141, 422–424.
Bjorklund, N., Nordlander, G. & Bylund, H. (2005) Olfactory and ̈
visual stimuli used in orientation to conifer seedlings by the pine
weevil, Hylobius abietis. Physiological Entomology, 30, 225–231.
Borden, J.H., Hunt, D.W.A., Miller, D.R. & Slessor, K.N. (1985)
Orientation in forest Coleoptera: an uncertain outcome of responses
by individual beetles to variable stimuli. Mechanisms in Insect
Olfaction (ed. by T. L. Payne, M. C. Birch and C. E. J. Kennedy),
pp. 97–109. Clarendon Press, U.K.
Campbell, S.A. & Borden, J.H. (2006a) Close-range, in-flight integra- tion of olfactory and visual information by a host-seeking bark beetle.
Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata, 120, 91–98.
Campbell, S.A. & Borden, J.H. (2006b) Integration of visual and
olfactory cues of hosts and non-hosts by three bark beetles
(Coleoptera: Scolytidae). Ecological Entomology, 31, 437–449.
Campbell, S.A. & Borden, J.H. (2009) Additive and synergistic
integration of multimodal cues of both hosts and non-hosts during
host selection by woodboring insects. Oikos, 118, 553–563.
Carmer, S.G. & Swanson, M.R. (1973) An evaluation of ten pairwise
multiple comparison procedures by Monte Carlo methods. Journal
of the American Statistical Association, 68, 66.
Cerezke, H. (1994) Warren rootcollar weevil, Hylobius warreni Wood
(Coleoptera: Curculionidae), in Canada: ecology, behavior, dam- age relationships, and management. Canadian Entomologist, 126,
1383–1442.
Claver, M.A. & Ambrose, D.P. (2001) Impact of antennectomy, eye
blinding and tibial comb coating on the predatory behaviour of
Rhynocoris kumarii Ambrose and Livingstone (Het., Reduviidae)
on Spodoptera litura Fabr. (Lep., Noctuidae). Journal of Applied
Entomology, 125, 519–525.
De Groot, P. & Nott, R. (2001) Evaluation of traps of six different
designs to capture pine sawyer beetles (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae).
Agricultural and Forest Entomology, 3, 107–111.
Dethier, V.G. (1983) Introduction. Herbivorous Insects: Host-Seeking
Behavior and Mechanisms (ed. by S. Ahmad), pp. 14–16. Academic
Press Inc., New York, New York.
Dubbel, V., Kerck, K., Sohrt, M. & Mangold, S. (1985) Influence
of trap color on the efficiency of bark beetle pheromone traps.
Zeitschrift fuer Angewandte Entomologie (Germany), 99, 59–64.
Duke, L. & Lindgren, B.S. (2006) Attack by Hylobius warreni on
grafted lodgepole pine and its relationships with monoterpene
composition and scion: rootstock diameter ratio. Agricultural and
Forest Entomology, 8, 305–311.
Gotelli, N.J. & Ellison, A.M. (2004) A Primer of Ecological Statistics.
Sinauer Associates Inc., Sunderland, Massachusetts.
Goyer, R.A., Lenhard, G.J. & Strom, B.L. (2004) The influence of
silhouette color and orientation on arrival and emergence of Ips pini
engravers and their predators in loblolly pine. Forest Ecology and
Management, 191, 147–155.
Grant, J. (1966) The hosts and distribution of the root weevils Hylobius
pinicola (Couper) and H. warreni Wood in British Columbia.
Journal of the Entomological Society of British Columbia, 63, 3–5.
Hoffman, G.D., Hunt, D.W.A., Salom, S.M. & Raffa, K.F. (1997)
Reproductive readiness and niche differences affect responses of
conifer root weevils (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) to simulated host
orders. Physiological and Chemical Ecology, 26, 91–100.
Hoover, S. (2000) What do weevils do all night? The adult feeding
and host selection behaviour and diurnal activity of Hylobius
warreni Wood (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Undergraduate Thesis,
University of Northern British Columbia.
Hopkins, G., Klingenberg, M.D. & Aukema, B.H. (2009) Temptations
of weevil: feeding and ovipositional behaviour of Hylobius warreni
Wood on host and nonhost bark in laboratory bioassays. Agricultural
and Forest Entomology, 11, 397–403.
Horning, J.A. & Lindgren, B.S. (2002) Stand conditions and tree
root dysfunction affecting susceptibility of lodgepole pine, Pinus
contorta var. latifolia engel. to attack by the Warren root collar
weevil, Hylobius warreni. Poster Presentation, IUFRO Working
Party 7.01.02-Tree Resistance to Insects, 10–14 June 2002. Flagstaff,
Arizona.
Huber, D.P.W., Gries, R., Borden, J.H. & Pierce, H.D., Jr (2000) A
survey of antennal responses by five species of coniferophagous bark
beetles (Coleoptera: Scolytidae) to bark volatiles of six species of
angiosperm trees. Chemoecology, 10, 103–113.
Hunt, D.W.A. & Raffa, K.F. (1989) Attraction of Hylobius radicis and
Pachylobius picivorus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) to ethanol and
turpentine in pitfall traps. Environmental Entomology, 18, 351–355.
Hunt, D.W.A. & Raffa, K.F. (1991) Orientation of Hylobius pales and
Pachylobius picivorus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) to visual cues.
Great Lakes Entomologist, 24, 225–229.
Klingenberg, M.D., Bjorklund, N. & Aukema, B.H. (2010a) Seeing the ̈
forest through the trees: differential dispersal of Hylobius warreni
within modified forest habitats. Environmental Entomology, 39,
898–906.
Klingenberg, M.D., Lindgren, B.S., Gillingham, M.P. & Aukema, B.H.
(2010b) Management response to one insect pest may increase vul- nerability to another. Journal of Applied Ecology, 47, 566–574.
Kral, K. (2008) Spatial vision in binocular and monocular common
field grasshoppers (Chorthippus brunneus). Physiological Entomol- ogy, 33, 233–237.
Machial, L.A., Lindgren, B.S., Steenweg, R. & Aukema, B.H. (2012)
Dispersal of Warren root collar weevil in three types of habitat.
Environmental Entomology, in press.
Minkenberg, O.P., Tatar, M. & Rosenheim, J.A. (1992) Egg load as
a major source of variability in insect foraging and oviposition
behavior. Oikos, 65, 134.
National Research Council of Canada (2011) Sunrise/Sunset Cal- culator. British Columbia [WWW document]. URL http://www.
nrc-cnrc.gc.ca/eng/services/hia/sunrise-sunset.html [accessed on 4
November 2011].
Nordenhem, H. & Eidmann, H.H. (1991) Response of the pine weevil
Hylobius abietis L. (Col., Curculionidae) to host volatiles in different
phases of its adult life cycle. Journal of Applied Entomology, 112,
353–358.
Nordenhem, H. & Nordlander, G. (1994) Olfactory oriented migration
through soil by root-living Hylobius abietis (L.) larvae (Col.,
Curculionidae). Journal of Applied Entomology, 117, 457–462.
Nordlander, G. (1987) A method for trapping Hylobius abietis (L.) with
a standardized bait and its potential for forecasting seedling damage.
Scandinavian Journal of Forest Research, 2, 199–213.
Nordlander, G., Nordenhem, H. & Bylund, H. (1997) Oviposition pat- terns of the pine weevil Hylobius abietis. Entomologia Experimen- talis et Applicata, 85, 1–9.
Ohrn, P., Klingenberg, M.D., Hopkins, G. & Bj ̈ orklund, N. (2008) ̈
Two non-destructive techniques for determining the sex of live adult
Hylobius warreni. Canadian Entomologist, 140, 617–620.
Prokopy, R.J. & Owens, E.D. (1978) Visual generalist with visual spe- cialist phytophagous insects: host selection behaviour and applica- tion to management. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata, 24,
609–620.
Prokopy, R.J. & Owens, E.D. (1983) Visual detection of plants by
herbivorous insects. Annual Review of Entomology, 28, 337–364.
© 2012 The Authors
Agricultural and Forest Entomology © 2012 The Royal Entomological Society, Agricultural and Forest Entomology, 14, 286–294
Page 9 of 9
294 L. A. Machial et al.
R Development Core Team (2011) R: A Language and Environment
for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing,
Austria [WWW document]. URL http://www.R-project.org [accessed
in September 2011].
Raffa, K.F. (2001) Mixed messages across multiple trophic levels:
the ecology of bark beetle chemical communication systems.
Chemoecology, 11, 49–65.
Raffa, K.F. & Hunt, D.W.A. (1988) Use of baited pitfall traps for mon- itoring Pales weevil, Hylobius pales (Coleoptera: Curculionidae).
Great Lakes Entomologist, 21, 123–125.
Reeves, J.L. (2011) Vision should not be overlooked as an important
sensory modality for finding host plants. Environmental Entomology,
40, 855–863.
Reeves, J.L., Lorch, P.D. & Kershner, M.W. (2009) Vision is important
for plant location by the phytophagous aquatic specialist Euhry- chiopsis lecontei Dietz (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Journal of Insect
Behavior, 22, 54–64.
Rieske, L. & Raffa, K.F. (1993) Potential use of baited pitfall traps in
monitoring pine root weevil, Hylobius pales, Pachylobius picivorus,
and Hylobius radicis (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) populations and
infestation levels. Journal of Economic Entomology, 86, 475–485.
Robert, J.A. & Lindgren, B.S. (2006) Relationships between root form
and growth, stability, and mortality in planted versus naturally
regenerated lodgepole pine in north-central British Columbia.
Canadian Journal of Forest Research, 36, 2642–2653.
Rossel, S. (1986) Binocular spatial localization in the praying mantis.
Journal of Experimental Biology, 120, 265–281.
Schroff, A.Z., Lindgren, B.S. & Gillingham, M.P. (2006) Random
acts of weevil: a spatial analysis of Hylobius warreni attack on
Pinus contorta var. latifolia in the sub-boreal spruce zone of
northern British Columbia. Forest Ecology and Management, 227,
42–49.
Stenberg, J. & Ericson, L. (2007) Visual cues override olfactory cues
in the host-finding process of the monophagous leaf beetle Altica
engstroemi. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata, 125, 81–88.
Strom, B.L., Roton, L.M., Goyer, R.A. & Meeker, J.R. (1999) Visual
and semiochemical disruption of host finding in the southern pine
beetle. Ecological Applications, 9, 1028–1038.
Strom, B.L., Goyer, R.A. & Shea, P.J. (2001) Visual and olfactory
disruption of orientation by the western pine beetle to attractant- baited traps. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata, 100, 63–67.
Tilles, D.A., Sjodin, K., Nordlander, G. & Eidmann, H.H. (1986a) Syn- ̈
ergism between ethanol and conifer host volatiles as attractants for
the pine weevil, Hylobius abietis (L.) (Coleoptera: Curculionidae).
Journal of Economic Entomology, 79, 970–973.
Tilles, D.A., Nordlander, G., Nordenhem, H., Eidmann, H.H., Wass- gren, A.B. & Bergstrom, G. (1986b) Increased release of host ̈
volatiles from feeding scars: a major cause of field aggregation in
the pine weevil Hylobius abietis (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Envi- ronmental Entomology, 15, 1040–1054.
Toivonen, R. & Viiri, H. (2006) Adult large pine weevils Hylobius
abietis feed on silver birch Betula pendula even in the presence of
conifer seedlings. Agricultural and Forest Entomology, 8, 121–128.
VanderSar, T.J.D. & Borden, J.H. (1977) Visual orientation of Pissodes
strobi Peck (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) in relation to host selection
behaviour. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 55, 2042–2049.
Walton, A. (2010) BC Ministry of Forest and Range. Provincial-Level
Projection of the Current Mountain Pine Beetle Outbreak, British
Columbia [WWW document]. URL http://www.for.gov.bc.ca/
hre/bcmpb/BCMPB.v7.BeetleProjection.Update.pdf [accessed on 12
March 2011].
Warner, R.E. (1966) A review of the Hylobius of North America, with
a new species injurious to slash pine (Coleoptera: Curculionidae).
The Coleopterists Bulletin, 20, 65–81.
Warren, G.L. (1956) The effect of some site factors on the abundance
of Hypomolyx piceus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Ecology, 37,
132–139.
Whitney, R.D. (1961) Root wounds and associated root rots of white
spruce. Forestry Chronicle, 37, 401–411.
Winder, L. (2004) Marking by abrasion or branding and recapturing
carabid beetles in studies of their movement. International Journal
of Pest Management, 50, 161–164.
Wood, S.L. (1957) The North American allies of Hylobius piceus
(De Geer) (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Canadian Entomologist, 89,
37–43.
Wood, C.S. & Van Sickle, G.A. (1989) Forest Insect and Disease Con- ditions British Columbia and Yukon 1989. Pacific Forest Research
Center, Canadian Forest Service, Environment Canada, British
Columbia.
Accepted 9 January 2012
First published online 22 March 2012
© 2012 The Authors
Agricultural and Forest Entomology © 2012 The Royal Entomological Society, Agricultural and Forest Entomology, 14, 286–294